翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Linguistic purism in the Korean language
・ Linguistic reconstruction
・ Linguistic relativity
・ Linguistic relativity and the color naming debate
・ Linguistic rights
・ Linguistic sequence complexity
・ Linguistic Society of America
・ Linguistic Society of Hong Kong
・ Linguistic Society of the Philippines
・ Linguistic Survey of India
・ Linguistic system
・ Linguistic Systems
・ Linguistic turn
・ Linguistic Typology
・ Linguistic typology
Linguistic universal
・ Linguistic validation
・ Linguistic value
・ Linguistic variety in Mauritius
・ Linguistics
・ Linguistics (journal)
・ Linguistics and Philosophy
・ Linguistics and the Book of Mormon
・ Linguistics Association of Great Britain
・ Linguistics in education
・ Linguistics of the Soviet Union
・ Linguistics Research Center at UT Austin
・ Linguistics Society of Iran
・ Linguistics Wars
・ Linguists' Club (London)


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Linguistic universal : ウィキペディア英語版
Linguistic universal
A linguistic universal is a pattern that occurs systematically across natural languages, potentially true for all of them. For example, ''All languages have nouns and verbs'', or ''If a language is spoken, it has consonants and vowels.'' Research in this area of linguistics is closely tied to the study of linguistic typology, and intends to reveal generalizations across languages, likely tied to cognition, perception, or other abilities of the mind. The field was largely pioneered by the linguist Joseph Greenberg, who derived a set of forty-five basic universals, mostly dealing with syntax, from a study of some thirty languages.
==Terminology==
Linguists distinguish between two kinds of universals: absolute (opposite: statistical, often called tendencies) and implicational (opposite non-implicational). Absolute universals apply to every known language and are quite few in number; an example is ''All languages have pronouns''. An implicational universal applies to languages with a particular feature that is always accompanied by another feature, such as ''If a language has trial grammatical number, it also has dual grammatical number'', while non-implicational universals just state the existence (or non-existence) of one particular feature.
Also in contrast to absolute universals are tendencies, statements that may not be true for all languages, but nevertheless are far too common to be the result of chance.〔Dryer (1998)〕 They also have implicational and non-implicational forms. An example of the latter would be ''The vast majority of languages have nasal consonants''.〔Lushootseed and Rotokas are examples of the rare languages which truly lack nasal consonants as normal speech sounds.〕 However, most tendencies, like their universal counterparts, are implicational. For example, ''With overwhelmingly greater-than-chance frequency, languages with normal SOV order are postpositional''. Strictly speaking, a tendency is not a kind of universal, but exceptions to most statements called universals can be found. For example, Latin is an SOV language with prepositions. Often it turns out that these exceptional languages are undergoing a shift from one type of language to another. In the case of Latin, its descendant Romance languages switched to SVO, which is a much more common order among prepositional languages.
Universals may also be bidirectional or unidirectional. In a bidirectional universal two features each imply the existence of each other. For example, languages with postpositions usually have SOV order, and likewise SOV languages usually have postpositions. The implication works both ways, and thus the universal is bidirectional. By contrast, in a unidirectional universal the implication works only one way. Languages that place relative clauses before the noun they modify again usually have SOV order, so pre-nominal relative clauses imply SOV. On the other hand, SOV languages worldwide show little preference for pre-nominal relative clauses, and thus SOV implies little about the order of relative clauses. As the implication works only one way, the proposed universal is a unidirectional one.
Linguistic universals in syntax are sometimes held up as evidence for universal grammar (although epistemological arguments are more common). Other explanations for linguistic universals have been proposed, for example, that linguistic universals tend to be properties of language that aid communication. If a language were to lack one of these properties, it has been argued, it would probably soon evolve into a language having that property.〔Daniel everett: Language the cultural tool〕
Michael Halliday has argued for a distinction between descriptive and theoretical categories in resolving the matter of the existence of linguistic universals, a distinction he takes from J.R. Firth and Louis Hjelmslev. He argues that "theoretical categories, and their inter-relations construe an abstract model of language...; they are interlocking and mutually defining". Descriptive categories, by contrast, are those set up to describe particular languages. He argues that "When people ask about 'universals', they usually mean descriptive categories that are assumed to be found in all languages. The problem is there is no mechanism for deciding how much alike descriptive categories from different languages have to be before they are said to be 'the same thing'" 〔Halliday, M.A.K. 2002. A personal perspective. In On Grammar, Volume 1 in the Collected Works of M.A.K. Halliday. London and New York: Continuumm p12.〕

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Linguistic universal」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.